Notepad with the numbers 2024 written on it, alongside a cup of coffee, a pair of glasses, and a pen.

Flex appeal: Sponteneity can be as important as planning New Year's Resolutions

New research suggests that people may not always want help with sticking to their New Year’s resolutions.

Individuals often make resolutions in January to maintain healthy lifestyle regimes – for example to eat better, stop smoking or exercise more often – then fail to keep them.

Behavioural scientists frequently interpret such behaviour as evidence of a conflict between the two ‘selves’ of a person – a 'planner' (in charge of self-control) and a 'doer' (who responds spontaneously to the temptations of the moment). 

A team of researchers from Warwick Business School, the Universities of East Anglia (UEA), Cardiff and Lancaster in the UK and Passau in Germany investigated how far people identify with their planners and their doers.

They found that while people differed in the relative importance they attached to spontaneity and self-control, overall, attitudes in favour of spontaneity were almost as common as people favouring self-control.

Public policies designed to ‘nudge’ people towards healthy lifestyles are often justified on the grounds that people think of their planners as their true selves and disown the actions of their doers. 

However, in their study Taking the New Year’s Resolution Test seriously: eliciting individuals’ judgements about self-control and spontaneity published in the journal Behavioural Public Policy, the authors argue this justification overlooks the possibility that people value spontaneity as well as self-control, and approve of their own flexible attitudes to resolutions.

Andrea Isoni, Professor of Behavioural Science at Warwick Business School, said: “We conclude that identifying when and where individuals want to be helped to avoid self-control failures is not as straightforward as many behavioural economists seem to think.

“We believe our findings point to the importance of treating desire for spontaneity as equally deserving of attention as the desire for self-control, and as suggesting interesting lines of further research.

“One idea it would be useful to investigate is whether some kinds of deviation from long-term goals are viewed as more spontaneity-affirming than others. For example, we found a contrast between our respondents’ spontaneity-favouring attitudes to sugary drinks and restaurant desserts and their desire for self-control to exercise. Breaking a health-oriented resolution by ordering a crème brûlée is perhaps a more positive way of expressing spontaneity than not taking one’s daily run on a wet day.”

Robert Sugden, Professor of Economics at UEA, said: “Our key message is not about whether nudges towards healthy lifestyles are good for people’s long-term health or happiness. It is about whether such nudges can be justified on the grounds that they help individuals to overcome what they themselves acknowledge as self-control problems. 

“If that idea is to be used as a guiding principle for public policy, we need to be assured that individuals want to be helped in this way. Our findings suggest that people often may not want this.”

The experiment, run via an online survey, began by asking each of the 240 participants to recall and write about a particular type of previous episode in their life. For some, this was a memorable meal when they had particularly enjoyed the food; for others, it was an effort they had made that was good for their health and they felt satisfied about.

They were then asked to say how well they recognised themselves in various statements. These included wishes for more self-control (such as ‘I wish I took more exercise’), regret about lapses of self-control (‘After ordering desserts in restaurants, I often feel regret’), and approval of self-control as a life strategy (‘In life it’s important to be able to resist temptation’). 

An equal number of statements expressed wishes for less self-control (such as ‘I wish there was less social pressure to take exercise’), regret about exercising self-control (‘After ordering a healthy dish, I often wish I’d chosen something tastier’), and approval of spontaneity (‘Having occasional treats is an important source of happiness for me, even if they are bad for my health’).

Overall, respondents recognised themselves almost as often in statements favouring spontaneity as in statements favouring self-control. In responding to statements about what was important in life, most participants maintained both; that it was important to make long-term plans and stick to them and that there was no harm in occasionally taking small enjoyments rather than sticking to those plans. Surprisingly, attitudes were not significantly affected by the type of episode respondents had recalled.

The research was supported by funding from the Economic and Social Research Council and the European Research Council under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme.

 

Andrea Isoni is Professor of Behavioural Science and teaches Foundations of Data Analysis for Management on BSc ManagementBSc International Management, and BSc Accounting & Finance.

Learn more about Behavioural Science on the four-day Executive Education course Behavioural Science in Consumers and Markets.

For more articles on Decision-Making and Analytics sign up to Core Insights here.